Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Cell Scientists to Watch
    • First Person
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JCS
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Fast-track manuscripts
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • JCS Prize
    • Manuscript transfer network
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JCS
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Cell Science
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Cell Science

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS   Twitter  Facebook   YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Cell Scientists to Watch
    • First Person
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JCS
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Fast-track manuscripts
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • JCS Prize
    • Manuscript transfer network
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JCS
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
Sticky Wicket
The opposite of eureka I: nurture
Mole
Journal of Cell Science 2005 118: 3567-3568; doi: 10.1242/jcs.02479
Mole
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Monday morning. Strong coffee, read the paper (the newspaper, it's Monday), brace myself. Deep breaths and procrastination (maybe just one more cup of coffee). Time to face the music.


Embedded Image

The weekend is over, and it is time to look at the crestfallen faces of all the Molets in the lab whose experiments not only failed, but failed on the weekend, thus reminding them so unkindly of what else they might have been doing to the same, ultimate experimental result.

What hurts more than a failed experiment from which nothing at all can be learnt (except that something is horribly wrong)? The mind leaps to thoughts like this: “Instead of spending the past 48 or so hours cooped up here, I could have been lounging on the deck of a fabulous yacht, sipping cocktails carried by gorgeous servers, while we gazed on the turquoise waters of where it is that I was not. And I'd still be just where I am now, only happy.” Technically, according to the laws of logic, if/then statements are automatically true when the `if' part is false: “if my experiment hadn't failed miserably, I would have instead just spent a weekend on some fabulous yacht” is logically true, but don't ask me why. It definitely feels true, though.

Experiments fail. It's a fact of science and a fact of life. In the facts of life category, here's one that always fails for me: go into a store to buy a gift without knowing what I'm going to buy – inevitable failure). But in science, experiments fail for a variety of reasons, and in biomedical science, the reasons can be so profound that it is amazing that things ever actually work. But we'll get to those profound reasons later; first, let's look at some relatively easy ones.

Think back to your lab practical days (for those of you who might be reading this who never took a laboratory course, I have this to say, “What in the world are you doing reading this? Go outside and play. You need the sun.”) Those were those horrid courses where you were instructed to perform an `experiment' for which the answer was already known (or readily calculated) and you were given a set amount of time in which to get that answer `experimentally' with the deviation from the expected results determining your grade. (I was honest, and therefore got terrible grades, like the time we were supposed to determine Planck's constant and my answer was `noxious smell'.) And I detested those vermin who fudged their results, `fudged' being a nice term for `faked'. But they got great grades and learnt a valuable lesson in the performance of scientific research (yes, I'm being sarcastic). Maybe you liked these courses (in which case, you too need to go play outside). I hated them, even the chemistry class in which we performed so many ether extractions that we ended up sitting on the floor giggling over how hopeless our measurements were (these were less enlightened times).

But I digress. The simple and obvious point is that even when we know the result of an `experiment' in advance (hence the quotation marks I have been using so annoyingly), they can fail for simple reasons of technical inexperience. There are a number of procedures we do routinely in my lab that rarely work the first time they are learned by a new Molet, but work reproducibly once performed a few times. These are among the easiest failures, which, while frustrating, evoke a knowing “Do it again, it'll work.”

A second class of experimental failure is similar, but can apply to anyone, even the most experienced bench scientist. These are failures as a consequence of random variables, which is the way we scientists say “crummy luck”. Any experiment worth doing is a complex process, and as we know, complexity generally invokes some level of chaos. A butterfly wing-flap on a beautiful South Pacific island (where you would have been, had your experiment not failed), etc., etc. You know, chaos. A bit of dust, a puff of air, a sudden attractive distraction at the wrong time – something goes wrong just this once. And the experienced among us know to just do it again, with a shrug and a “it happens.” Minimizing chaos is what we do, but we can't do it all the time. (As for me, my desk is approaching entropic heat death, but that's just me).

There is a third source of failure that is an uncomfortable one, but as with the first two, not terribly interesting. But it is disturbing (for us withered old folks) and becoming a more important source of failure than the first two (which are solved simply by `doing it again'). You bought an antibody, a reagent, a kit – and it didn't work. Of course, you won't know that this is why your experiment failed until you've spent about a week (or more) checking every antibody, reagent, and kit that contributed to your massive experimental undertaking, whereupon you discover that the culprit was a commercially obtained thingie that doesn't do its thing, despite aggressive advertising to the contrary.

Why is this so prevalent? Surely, no respectable purveyor of quality merchandise for the research community would intentionally (or even unintentionally) sell us something that doesn't work: that goes against the central tenets of capitalism and the force of the marketplace (for an American, like me, tantamount to heresy). But sadly, this is so, and it is becoming decreasingly uncommon (it's a sad thing, which is why I used the embedded double negative to try to cheer you up).

It is worth taking a moment to see why this is so, which will lead us to undertake the only way we can go about avoiding this important source of failure. Companies produce reagents and kits to sell, and if they never work at all (or only rarely) the company will eventually cease to exist. Usually this ensures that, at least at first, things work – a reagent begins its commercial life as a useful (or moderately useful) commodity. (Some reagents are notoriously useless from the start, and it seems that it is entirely by the force of advertising that they continue to be used, thereby contributing either directly to failure or to the constant trickle of published artifacts.)

But good reagents often go awry, and the dependable kit can suddenly become the nail for which the battle was lost. There is a fundamental reason for this. Individuals who succeed in making useful reagents of excellent and reproducible quality for a company are generally rewarded by being given the more interesting task of making new products; the job of keeping up the quality of the old ones falls to others who are not always as conscientious as the originator. Quality declines. Experiments fail.

There is a simple lesson here, proving the maxim that even a terrible experiment has value if only to serve as a bad example (we have many of these displayed around the lab with appropriate and often funny labels, such as “boil your probes!”, and these have the added fun of causing utterly confused looks on our non-science visitors). It is this: test your reagents before you use them in an experiment. A good experiment is a hugely expensive undertaking, expensive not only in terms of reagents and time, but the tremendously precious commodity of expectation. We expect a well-designed experiment to yield useful information (hence the investment of reagents and time); when they don't, we lose momentum. We stop loving our question and it becomes an albatross around our necks (data, data, everywhere, and not a byte to eat – sorry STC). Yes, it's boring to test each reagent in simple assays, determining optimal concentrations and procedures for each new batch of even our trusted reagents, but in doing so we can greatly increase the chances that the all important experiment will work (and work repeatedly). Untried experiments, crafted in our imaginations, are infants. We have to test their food and the temperature of the bathwater, and protect them from the cruel world around them. Nurture them. Because when they do succeed, we are so proud of them, and we can't wait to show pictures of them to our friends and even total strangers.

To this point, however, we haven't talked about the most insidious reason why experiments fail. A reason that is at once terrible and wonderful. But not right now – I've been avoiding facing the Molets...

  • © The Company of Biologists Limited 2005
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Cell Science.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The opposite of eureka I: nurture
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Cell Science
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Cell Science web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Sticky Wicket
The opposite of eureka I: nurture
Mole
Journal of Cell Science 2005 118: 3567-3568; doi: 10.1242/jcs.02479
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Sticky Wicket
The opposite of eureka I: nurture
Mole
Journal of Cell Science 2005 118: 3567-3568; doi: 10.1242/jcs.02479

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Corona XXX – speechless
  • Corona XXIX – four minutes
  • Corona XXVIII – the Rashomon gate
Show more STICKY WICKET

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Experimental Biology

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Introducing FocalPlane’s new Community Manager, Esperanza Agullo-Pascual

We are pleased to welcome Esperanza to the Journal of Cell Science team. The new Community Manager for FocalPlane, Esperanza is joining us from the Microscopy Core at Mount Sinai School of Medicine. Find out more about Esperanza in her introductory post over on FocalPlane.


New funding scheme supports sustainable events

As part of our Sustainable Conferencing Initiative, we are pleased to announce funding for organisers that seek to reduce the environmental footprint of their event. The next deadline to apply for a Scientific Meeting grant is 26 March 2021.


Read & Publish participation continues to grow

"Alongside pre-printing for early documentation of work, such mechanisms are particularly helpful for early-career researchers like me.”

Dr Chris MacDonald (University of York) shares his experience of publishing Open Access as part of our growing Read & Publish initiative. We now have over 150 institutions in 15 countries and four library consortia taking part – find out more and view our full list of participating institutions.


Cell scientist to watch: Romain Levayer

In an interview, Romain Levayer talks about starting his own lab, his love for preprints and his experience of balancing parenting with his research goals.


Live lactating mammary tissue

In a stunning video, Stewart et al. demonstrate warping of the alveolar unit due to basal cell-generated force as part of their recent work investigating roles for mechanically activated ion channels in lactation and involution.

Visit our YouTube channel to watch more videos from JCS, our sister journals and the Company.


JCS and COVID-19

For more information on measures Journal of Cell Science is taking to support the community during the COVID-19 pandemic, please see here.

If you have any questions or concerns, please do not hestiate to contact the Editorial Office.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About Journal of Cell Science
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Fast-track manuscripts
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • JCS Prize
  • Manuscript transfer network
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contacts

  • Contact JCS
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992